Implicit attitudes can change over the long term

January 07, 2019

Data from more than 4 million tests completed between 2004 and 2016 show that Americans' attitudes toward certain social groups are becoming less biased over time, according to research published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science. The findings show that participants' self-reported (explicit) attitudes regarding groups defined by age, disability, body weight, race, skin tone, and sexuality have all shifted toward neutrality over the span of a decade. Crucially, the data also showed that participants' more automatic (implicit) attitudes toward race, skin tone, and sexuality have also decreased in bias over time.

"We provide the first report of long-term change in both implicit and explicit attitudes - measured from the same individual - towards multiple social groups," says psychological scientist Tessa E. S. Charlesworth of Harvard University, first author on the study. "This research is important because it shows that, contrary to previous assumptions that implicit attitudes were stable features of the mind or society, implicit attitudes appear, in fact, to be capable of long-term durable change."

Charlesworth and Harvard University psychology professor Mahzarin R. Banaji used statistical models similar to those employed by economic forecasters to analyze change in data collected via the Project Implicit website from January 1, 2004 to December 31, 2016. Only participants living in the United States were included in the analyses.

To gauge participants' automatic, or implicit, attitudes toward different groups, the researchers used variations of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). For each social group examined in this study, participants completed a computerized IAT task in which they pressed a specific computer key to categorize pairs of words related to social groups and attributes. In the age IAT, for example, the researchers compared how long it took participants to respond when young and good had the same key and old and bad had the same key with how long it took them to respond when young and bad and old and good had the same keys. In general, faster responses to certain pairings in the IAT are thought to reflect a stronger automatic association between the concepts and attributes in those pairings.

For each social group, participants also provided self-reported ratings intended to measure their explicit attitudes toward the group.

In line with broad cultural trends, participants' explicit attitudes toward the social groups showed decreasing bias over time:

"Our analyses revealed that all self-reported attitudes about age, disability, body weight, race, skin tone, and sexuality have moved towards less prejudice over the past decade," Charlesworth explains. "The rates of explicit attitude change ranged from a rapid 49% decrease in explicit bias towards gay and lesbian individuals, to a relatively slower 15% decrease in explicit bias towards overweight individuals."

And participants' implicit associations also showed notable long-term change:

"Implicit sexual orientation, race, and skin-tone attitudes have all decreased in prejudice over the past decade, with the rates of change ranging from a rapid 33% decrease in implicit bias towards gay and lesbian individuals to a relatively slower 15% decrease in implicit bias towards dark-skinned individuals," says Charlesworth.

The change in implicit attitudes related to sexual orientation emerged across all generations and demographic groups, whereas change in implicit race and skin-tone attitudes appeared strongest among millennials and varied according to participants' race.

The trend toward neutrality in implicit attitudes did not extend to all of the social groups, however. For example, implicit attitudes toward elderly individuals and disabled individuals appeared to be relatively stable over time. And implicit attitudes toward overweight individuals even showed a slight shift toward increased bias over time.

"Because implicit attitudes exist largely outside of conscious awareness and are relatively difficult to control, the assumption has been that they will not change or, at the least, will change very slowly," Charlesworth notes. These analyses provide evidence to the contrary, showing that population-level implicit attitudes can indeed change over time.

One outstanding question is why some attitudes, such as sexuality attitudes, appear to be changing so much faster than others, such as age attitudes. Charlesworth and Banaji plan on continuing to track attitudes toward different social groups, examining how the trends predicted by their current analyses align with actual data collected in 2017, 2018, and beyond. Such comparisons can reveal how significant social events, such as elections and new social policies, may play a role in shaping population-level attitudes on both explicit and implicit measures.

"Answering this question can help guide applied discussions on the design of policies or interventions that serve to move all attitudes - towards all social groups on both implicit and explicit measures - in the direction of greater equity," Charlesworth concludes.
-end-
Deidentified and cleaned data for this study, along with data-analysis scripts, have been made publicly available via the Open Science Framework. The raw deidentified data and the materials from the Project Implicit demonstration website database are archived online. This article has received the badges for Open Data and Open Materials.

For more information about this study, please contact: Tessa Charlesworth at tet371@g.harvard.edu.

The article abstract is available online at https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0956797618813087

The APS journal Psychological Science is the highest ranked empirical journal in psychology. For a copy of the article "Patterns of Implicit and Explicit Attitudes: I. Long-Term Change and Stability From 2007 to 2016" and access to other Psychological Science research findings, please contact Anna Mikulak at 202-293-9300 or amikulak@psychologicalscience.org.

Association for Psychological Science

Related Body Weight Articles from Brightsurf:

Weight gainers more likely to underestimate their true body size
People with obesity who gain weight have a tendency to perceive their own body size as smaller than it actually is compared to those who maintain a stable weight, according to new research following more than 2,000 people with obesity from the Swedish Obese Subjects (SOS) study over 10 years.

Body weight has surprising, alarming impact on brain function
Amsterdam and Costa Mesa, CA, August 5, 2020 - As a person's weight goes up, all regions of the brain go down in activity and blood flow, according to a new brain imaging study in the Journal of Alzheimer's Disease.

Brain insulin sensitivity determines body weight and fat distribution
Just where fat is deposited in the body and to what degree a person may benefit from a lifestyle intervention depends on how sensitive the brain is to insulin.

Examining association between childhood video game use, adolescent body weight
Researchers looked at whether there was a long-term association between using video games at an early age and later weight as a teenager, as well as what role behaviors such as physical activity, the regularity of bedtimes and consuming sugar-sweetened beverages might play.

Brain receptor that regulates body heat may also help accelerate weight loss
The brain mechanism that enables us to maintain a constant body temperature may also be the key to rapid weight loss, a new study finds.

Short-term study suggests vegan diet can boost gut microbes related to body weight, body composition and blood sugar control
New research presented at this year's Annual Meeting of the European Association for the Study of Diabetes (EASD) in Barcelona, Spain (Sept.

Increased body weight in adolescent boys linked with heart attack before 65
A study in nearly 1.7 million 18-year-old boys has found that higher body mass index (BMI) is linked with greater risk of a heart attack before 65 years of age.

Excess weight and body fat cause cardiovascular disease
In the first Mendelian randomization study to look at this, researchers have found evidence that excess weight and body fat cause a range of heart and blood vessel diseases (rather than just being associated with it).

New brain mechanisms regulating body weight
Researchers at University of Gothenburg, Sweden, clarify the link between the molecule interleukine-6 (IL-6) in the brain and obesity.

Excess body weight before 50 is associated with higher risk of dying from pancreatic cancer
Excess weight before age 50 may be more strongly associated with pancreatic cancer mortality risk than excess weight at older ages, according to results of a study presented at the AACR Annual Meeting 2019, March 29-April 3.

Read More: Body Weight News and Body Weight Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.