Nav: Home

When it comes to mating, fruit flies can make rational choices

January 17, 2017

Humans make rational choices -- though perhaps not all the time. But does the ability for rational decision-making extend to other members of the animal kingdom? If so, how far are they from the human lineage?

The answer, according to researchers from the University of Washington, is pretty far.

In a paper published Jan. 17 in the journal Nature Communications, they report that fruit flies -- perhaps the most widely studied insect in history -- show signs of rational decision-making when choosing a mate. Through a complex series of behavioral experiments, the team shows that male fruit flies, when presented with a pair of females as potential mating partners, display a key component of rational choice: transitivity.

"Transitivity is a hallmark of rational decision-making," said senior author Daniel Promislow, a UW professor of pathology and biology. "Essentially, it is the process of establishing a rank order of preference, and then making behavioral decisions based on that hierarchy."

Transitivity has been shown in other animals, such as some bird species, while searching for food. But Promislow's team, led by first author and postdoctoral researcher Devin Arbuthnott, is among the first to see if rationality extends to mate choice.

The researchers showed that individual male fruit flies from one wild-derived strain, called Canton-S, displayed transitivity when presented with potential female mates from 10 different laboratory strains of fruit flies. In these tests, a researcher would place one Canton-S male in an arena with a pair of females, each from a different strain, and note if the male mated with either female.

"Before each test, we would mark each female with a yellow or red fluorescent powder," said Arbuthnott, who is now a postdoctoral researcher at the University of British Columbia. "If the male attempted to mate with either female, we could determine the female's identity based on her color."

To account for all possible pairings of females from the 10 strains, they tested Canton-S males against 45 pairs of females. No fly, male or female, was tested more than once. After repeating these tests 10 to 20 times for each combination, the researchers discovered that Canton-S males displayed a consistent, ranked preference regarding which female to mate with.

"This is the pattern we would expect to see if males are making transitive decisions -- a sign of rational choice," said Arbuthnott.

Using the same assays, they showed that males from a second strain, Oregon-R, also display transitivity with females from the 10 laboratory strains. There were only a few small differences between the hierarchies displayed by Oregon-R males and Canton-S males, said Arbuthnott.

Though the researchers only tested mate choice in male flies, the decision to mate in this species is definitely a two-way street. But Arbuthnott focused on male mate choice in these experiments to help dispel a misconception about mating in many animal species.

"There is a classic theory that females are the 'choosy' sex and males aren't choosy," said Arbuthnott. "We wanted to show that males are definitely making choices too when interacting with the females."

The males are likely responding to a combination of visual, chemical and behavioral cues from the females. The team conducted more experiments to learn about the "information" the males picked up from females. Blind males still displayed transitivity when choosing between females, as did mutant males who had no sense of taste and smell. But blind males with no sense of smell or taste did not display transitivity.

"The results from these sensory deprivation experiments indicate that there is some redundancy in the information provided by the female visual and chemical cues," said Promislow.

They also analyzed one particular signal, a complex secretion of chemicals known as cuticular hydrocarbons, or CHCs.

"CHCs are essentially 'contact pheromones,'" said Arbuthnott. "When they're in close proximity, fruit flies can taste one another's secretions and -- we believe -- learn information about a potential mate."

By gas chromatography, which separates and identifies individual CHC molecules, they discovered that females from lines less likely to be chosen for mating secreted higher levels of two particular CHCs, which may act as "repellant" signals.

In addition, the researchers counted the number of offspring produced by females from each line, and discovered that males were more likely to mate with females with a greater capacity to produce the next generation.

Their results indicate that male fruit flies construct a hierarchy -- a sign of transitivity and rational choice -- and may be doing so by integrating diverse visual and chemical cues from females.

"They're able to process the information they're receiving in the most advantageous way," said Arbuthnott. "Their decisions are transitive, which is indicative of rational choice."
-end-
Co-authors are Tatyana Fedina and Scott Pletcher with the University of Michigan. The research was funded by the National Institutes of Health.

For more information, contact Promislow at 206-616-6994 or promislo@uw.edu and Arbuthnott at 604-356-4714 or darbuth@zoology.ubc.ca.

DOI: 10.1038/NCOMMS13953

Grant number: GM102279.

University of Washington

Related Fruit Flies Articles:

Fruit flies respond to rapid changes in the visual environment
Researchers have discovered a mechanism employed by the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster that broadens our understanding of visual perception.
How fruit flies flock together in orderly clusters
Opposing desires to congregate and maintain some personal space drive fruit flies to form orderly clusters, according to a study published today in eLife.
Fruit flies help in the development of personalized medicine
It is common knowledge that there is a connection between our genes and the risk of developing certain diseases.
Fruit flies' microbiomes shape their evolution
In just five generations, an altered microbiome can lead to genome-wide evolution in fruit flies, according to new research led researchers at the University of Pennsylvania.
Why fruit flies eat practically anything
Kyoto University researchers uncover why some organisms can eat anything -- 'generalists -- and others have strict diets -- 'specialists'.
Why so fly: MU scientists discover some fruit flies learn better than others
Fruit flies could one day provide new avenues to discover additional genes that contribute to a person's ability to learn and remember.
Fruit flies find their way by setting navigational goals
Navigating fruit flies do not have the luxury of GPS, but they do have a kind of neural compass.
Tolerance to stress is a 'trade-off' as fruit flies age
With the help of the common fruit fly (D. melanogaster), which ages quickly because it only lives about 60 days, FAU neuroscientists provide insights into healthy aging by investigating the effects of a foraging gene on age and stress tolerance.
Fat fruit flies: High-sugar diet deadens sweet tooth; promotes overeating, obesity in flies
Some research suggests that one reason people with obesity overeat is because they don't enjoy food -- especially sweets -- as much as lean people.
Fruit flies help to shed light on the evolution of metabolism
Researchers at the University of Helsinki have discovered that the ability to use sugar as food varies strongly between closely related fruit fly species.
More Fruit Flies News and Fruit Flies Current Events

Trending Science News

Current Coronavirus (COVID-19) News

Top Science Podcasts

We have hand picked the top science podcasts of 2020.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Climate Mindset
In the past few months, human beings have come together to fight a global threat. This hour, TED speakers explore how our response can be the catalyst to fight another global crisis: climate change. Guests include political strategist Tom Rivett-Carnac, diplomat Christiana Figueres, climate justice activist Xiye Bastida, and writer, illustrator, and artist Oliver Jeffers.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#562 Superbug to Bedside
By now we're all good and scared about antibiotic resistance, one of the many things coming to get us all. But there's good news, sort of. News antibiotics are coming out! How do they get tested? What does that kind of a trial look like and how does it happen? Host Bethany Brookeshire talks with Matt McCarthy, author of "Superbugs: The Race to Stop an Epidemic", about the ins and outs of testing a new antibiotic in the hospital.
Now Playing: Radiolab

Speedy Beet
There are few musical moments more well-worn than the first four notes of Beethoven's Fifth Symphony. But in this short, we find out that Beethoven might have made a last-ditch effort to keep his music from ever feeling familiar, to keep pushing his listeners to a kind of psychological limit. Big thanks to our Brooklyn Philharmonic musicians: Deborah Buck and Suzy Perelman on violin, Arash Amini on cello, and Ah Ling Neu on viola. And check out The First Four Notes, Matthew Guerrieri's book on Beethoven's Fifth. Support Radiolab today at Radiolab.org/donate.