Beyond The "Science Wars:" Should "Non-Experts" Participate In The Scientific Enterprise?

February 18, 1997

What role should non-scientists play in regulating genetic engineering? Who should decide where federal research funds will be invested? What role should laypeople play in designing clinical trials for AIDS drugs?

Questions about the role of "non-experts" in science and technology policymaking and scientific practice underlie the so-called "science wars" now raging between outspoken scientists, social scientists and citizen activists.

On one side are those who believe that only "experts" are qualified to participate in the activities of the scientific community. On the other are those who believe that laypeople can and should be involved in practices as wide ranging as setting federal research priorities and cooperatively undertaking research with scientists in areas that affect their lives.

"Science and technology are important parts of virtually every American's life," said Dr. Daniel Lee Kleinman, assistant professor in the School of History, Technology and Society at the Georgia Institute of Technology. "We must examine whether scientists alone should make decisions about the trajectory of the scientific enterprise."

To encourage wider discussion of the issue, Kleinman has organized a session called "Science and Democracy: Beyond the 'Science Wars'" at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) February 13-18 in Seattle. The session, scheduled for 8:30 - 11:30 a.m. on February 18, will feature a range of viewpoints. (See list of panelists.)

So far, the "science wars" have largely been fought in the academic world, but Kleinman believes the difficult funding decisions to be made in Washington will soon raise the stakes. Unless consideration is given to broadening the role of non-experts in the U.S. science and technology enterprise, he warns, there could be serious consequences.

"Science and technology, and academic scholarship in general, are facing severe budgetary constraints as a result of the budget crises in Washington and the 50 states," Kleinman noted. "If scientists appear arrogant and assume that they can talk at, and not with, laypeople, they may find financial support drying up."

That would be unfortunate, because he believes many scientists are willing to accept informed input from "non-experts."

And, Kleinman adds, "there is room for laypeople to be involved in decision making about science and technology. There are cases that show, given the opportunity, laypeople can learn enough to make intelligent decisions about what research should be funded and even about how to do research.

"At a time when the growth of federal financial support for science has stagnated, the plausibility of this kind of decision making is what we ought to be talking about."

Kleinman notes that differing expectations, education and traditions have created a wide gulf between scientists and laypeople.

"Scientists are educated to think about their roles very differently than non-scientists," he explained. "Scientists are taught to believe in the notion of expertise and the sharp distinction between expert and non-expert."

As a result, scientists may question whether "non-experts" can make worthwhile contributions to substantive debate on science and technology issues. Surveys showing the general public with a low level of scientific literacy reinforce those doubts.

And because the pursuit of advanced scientific degrees require many years of study and training, scientists may logically wonder how laypeople could gain enough knowledge to discuss complex issues.

On the other side, laypeople often assume they cannot comprehend scientific issues. As a result, they may be content to leave complex technical questions to the experts, believing "scientists know what's best," Kleinman notes.

Institutional issues also create obstacles to establishing mechanisms for cooperation between scientists and laypeople, he points out.

Faculty contributions to public debate and communication about science and technology are seldom factors used in making promotion and tenure decisions at colleges and universities. Such activities typically do not receive funding, and so take time away from teaching and research activities that are rewarded and supported by academic institutions.

"The long term health of the scientific community should create a huge incentive for scientists to be involved in creating avenues for lay participation in the scientific enterprise," Kleinman explained. "But in the short term, for individual scientists at individual institutions, there may not be many incentives."

Likewise, few citizens can afford to spend the time necessary to gain expertise in complex and controversial issues such as nuclear waste disposal or genetically-engineered growth hormones. Yet the consequences of these decisions affect everyone regardless of their expertise.

Kleinman believes that democratizing science will require new policies and programs to encourage cooperation between experts and non-experts. University tenure decisions might take more seriously scientist work with community groups, and "citizen sabbaticals" could offer laypeople paid work leaves to study issues and join in debates.

"We live in a democracy, and living in a democracy doesn't just mean going to the polls and voting every two years," he added. "It means civic participation in the kinds of decisions that affect our everyday lives."

# # #
Following are the panelists at the AAAS session organized by Kleinman:




RESEARCH COMMUNICATIONS OFFICE
223 Centennial Research Building
Georgia Institute of Technology
Atlanta, Georgia 30332-0828
MEDIA RELATIONS CONTACTS:
John Toon (404-894-6986) or Amanda Crowell (404-894-6980);
Internet: john.toon@gtri.gatech.edu; FAX: (404-894-6983)
TECHNICAL:
Dr. Daniel Kleinman (404-894-8401), Internet: daniel.kleinman@hts.gatech.edu.


WRITER: John Toon


Georgia Institute of Technology

Related Decision Making Articles from Brightsurf:

Knowing the model you can trust - the key to better decision-making
As much of Europe is engulfed by a second wave of Covid-19, and track and trace struggles to meet demand, modelling support tools are being increasingly used by policymakers to make key decisions.

Happy endings trip up the brain's decision-making
The brain keeps track of the value of an experience as well as how it unfolds over time; overemphasizing the ending may trigger poor decision-making, according to new research published in JNeurosci.

Automatic decision-making prevents us harming others - new study
The processes our brains use to avoid harming other people are automatic and reflexive - and quite different from those used when avoiding harm to ourselves, according to new research.

Mapping the decision-making pathways in the brain
Scientists at the Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University (OIST) have identified a new area of the brain that could be involved in cost-benefit decision-making.

How the brain's internal states affect decision-making
By recording the activity of separate populations of neurons simultaneously, researchers have gained an unprecedented insight into how the 'waxing and waning' of our mental state influences the decisions we make.

Motherhood overrides the brain's decision-making
Motherhood takes over the brain's decision-making regions to prioritize caring for offspring, according to new research in rats published in eNeuro.

Get it over with, or procrastinate? New research explores our decision-making process
New research from the UBC Sauder School of Business may have figured out why.

Illuminating interactions between decision-making and the environment
Employing a game theory model, University of Pennsylvania researchers demonstrate how strategic decisions influence the environment in which those decisions are made, alterations which in turn influence strategy.

Lung cancer screening decision aid delivered through tobacco quitlines improves informed decision-making
Researchers from The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center have shown that a decision aid delivered through tobacco quitlines effectively reaches a screening-eligible population and results in informed decisions about lung cancer screening.

A molecular map of the brain's decision-making area
Researchers at Karolinska Institutet have come one step closer toward understanding how the part of our brain that is central for decision-making and the development of addiction is organized on a molecular level.

Read More: Decision Making News and Decision Making Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.