Nav: Home

As genetic data expand, researchers urge caution in how predictors of education outcomes are used

February 20, 2019

WASHINGTON, D.C., February 20, 2019--Driven by the mapping of the human genome, research over the past decade has identified thousands of genetic differences among individuals that are associated with a wide variety of differences on key outcomes, ranging from blood pressure and height to depression and, perhaps surprisingly, educational attainment.It has long been known that the number of years a person spends in school is heritable. More recent research focuses on the links between variation in specific genetic markers and educational attainment. After identification of such markers, follow-up work has examined associations between differences in these genetic markers and differences in the development of traits such as cognitive ability, school performance, verbal ability, and learning disabilities. A 2018 study found that a composite summary of an individual's genetic makeup, a "polygenic score," explains roughly 12 percent of variation in years of schooling and 9 percent of variation in cognitive ability.

In a review published online today in AERA Open, a peer-reviewed journal of the American Educational Research Association, researchers from Stanford University and the University of Cambridge warn that--as the predictive power of genes tied to learning and educational outcomes increases and access to genetic data expands--researchers, educators, and policymakers must be cautious in how they use such data, interpret related findings, and, in the not-too-distant future, apply genetics-informed student interventions.

The review, titled "Genetics and Education: Recent Developments in the Context of an Ugly History and an Uncertain Future," was authored by Daphne Martschenko, a PhD candidate at the University of Cambridge; Sam Trejo, a doctoral candidate at Stanford University; and Benjamin W. Domingue, an assistant professor of education at Stanford University.

"The field of molecular genetics is moving fast and the floodgates of genetic data have opened," said Domingue. "It is important that researchers, policymakers, practitioners, and the public be aware of the limitations of genetic data and the corresponding dangers of misuse."

"In the past, genetic data were used to deny children opportunities for educational growth, erode public support for social policies, or propagate racist or classist beliefs," Domingue said. "Genetic data are a technological innovation that may allow for improvements in the human condition, but it is critical that we ensure such innovations don't come at the expense of affording all children the opportunity to learn."

The authors note that genetic research, as part of an "ugly history" of bigoted ideologies, was used to bolster unjust policies in the United States. The use of genetic language to describe racial and socioeconomic differences in cognitive ability and academic performance was commonplace in the 19th and 20th centuries and continues today, according to the research team.

The authors also note that:
  • Research on human genetics might offer information about developmental trajectories of traits--such as ADHD, dyslexia, and other learning disabilities--that are relevant to education. In the near term, such information may offer insight into strategies for intervention. In the longer term, it may even be useful for individual-level risk prediction. However, there is no evidence to suggest that any child cannot thrive in the appropriate environmental setting--and there is no research that justifies depriving children of needed interventions.

  • Genetic inheritance is not destiny. An increased understanding of how one's genetic inheritance influences development does not change the fact that environments play a massive role in educational attainment and success. Genes are implicated in many developmental processes, but environments have large effects on all people independent of their genetic makeup and may even moderate the role of genetic influences in some cases. Helping those students most at risk for health or sociobehavioral problems is one of the most pressing demands in education, irrespective of why such students are at risk. Providing an environment rich with educational possibilities for all children should always be a top priority.

  • Genetics may provide valuable insight into educational processes, but it cannot invalidate the importance of well-crafted public policy needed to support educators determined to address the collective task of educating the next generation.
While the predictive power of genetic data is still too weak to be used for individual-level interventions, such approaches--involving, perhaps, specialized interventions for a child with a genetic profile that suggests possible difficulties with reading--will likely be in demand someday soon, according to the authors.

"At some point in the future, parents of a young student may show up in a principal's office, armed with genetic data indicating that their child is at a heightened risk for dyslexia, and ask that learning supports be put into place immediately," said Domingue. "In envisioning this scenario, many questions come up. Do such learning supports exist? Do we know them to be effective? And might they be useful for all students?"

As genetic data become more commercialized, with higher income people having greater access to the information, educators and policymakers will have to consider the implications of using genetic predictors for both efficacy and equity, according to the researchers.

Equity concerns are especially relevant since genetic prediction can vary by ancestry. Research has found that the predictive power for European ancestry-derived polygenic scores can be lower in people of non-European ancestry. To date, research on genetic effects has been conducted largely on people of European ancestry.

"Will all children be able to take advantage of any useful diagnostic information that becomes available?" asked Domingue. "We would argue that to ensure that genetics research benefits all and not just some, regulation meant to safeguard privacy and equal treatment will be increasingly important."
-end-
About AERA

The American Educational Research Association (AERA) is the largest national interdisciplinary research association devoted to the scientific study of education and learning. Founded in 1916, AERA advances knowledge about education, encourages scholarly inquiry related to education, and promotes the use of research to improve education and serve the public good. Find AERA on Facebook, Twitter, and Instagram.

Contacts:

Tony Pals, tpals@aera.net
(202) 238-3235, (202) 288-9333 (cell)

Collin Boylin, cboylin@aera.net
(202) 238-3233, (860) 490-8326 (cell)

American Educational Research Association

Related Learning Articles:

Learning with light: New system allows optical 'deep learning'
A team of researchers at MIT and elsewhere has come up with a new approach to complex computations, using light instead of electricity.
Mount Sinai study reveals how learning in the present shapes future learning
The prefrontal cortex shapes memory formation by modulating hippocampal encoding.
Better learning through zinc?
Zinc is a vital micronutrient involved in many cellular processes: For example, in learning and memory processes, it plays a role that is not yet understood.
Deep learning and stock trading
A study undertaken by researchers at the School of Business and Economics at Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU) has shown that computer programs that algorithms based on artificial intelligence are able to make profitable investment decisions.
Learning makes animals intelligent
The fact that animals can use tools, have self-control and certain expectations of life can be explained with the help of a new learning model for animal behavior.
Learning Morse code without trying
Researchers at the Georgia Institute of Technology have developed a system that teaches people Morse code within four hours using a series of vibrations felt near the ear.
The adolescent brain is adapted to learning
Teenagers are often portrayed as seeking immediate gratification, but new work suggests that their sensitivity to reward could be part of an evolutionary adaptation to learn from their environment.
The brain watched during language learning
Researchers from Nijmegen, the Netherlands, have for the first time captured images of the brain during the initial hours and days of learning a new language.
Learning in the absence of external feedback
Rewards act as external factors that influence and reinforce learning processes.
New learning procedure for neural networks
Neural networks learn to link temporally dispersed stimuli.

Related Learning Reading:

Best Science Podcasts 2019

We have hand picked the best science podcasts for 2019. Sit back and enjoy new science podcasts updated daily from your favorite science news services and scientists.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Setbacks
Failure can feel lonely and final. But can we learn from failure, even reframe it, to feel more like a temporary setback? This hour, TED speakers on changing a crushing defeat into a stepping stone. Guests include entrepreneur Leticia Gasca, psychology professor Alison Ledgerwood, astronomer Phil Plait, former professional athlete Charly Haversat, and UPS training manager Jon Bowers.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#524 The Human Network
What does a network of humans look like and how does it work? How does information spread? How do decisions and opinions spread? What gets distorted as it moves through the network and why? This week we dig into the ins and outs of human networks with Matthew Jackson, Professor of Economics at Stanford University and author of the book "The Human Network: How Your Social Position Determines Your Power, Beliefs, and Behaviours".