Nav: Home

Study analyzes what 'a' and 'the' tell us about language acquisition

April 05, 2017

If you have the chance, listen to a toddler use the words "a" and "the" before a noun. Can you detect a pattern? Is he or she using those two words correctly?

And one more question: When kids start using language, how much of their know-how is intrinsic, and how much is acquired by listening to others speak?

Now a study co-authored by an MIT professor uses a new approach to shed more light on this matter -- a central issue in the area of language acquisition.

The results suggest that experience is an important component of early-childhood language usage although it doesn't necessarily account for all of a child's language facility. Moreover, the extent to which a child learns grammar by listening appears to change over time, with a large increase occurring around age 2 and a leveling off taking place in subsequent years.

"In this view, adult-like, rule-based [linguistic] development is the end-product of a construction of knowledge," says Roger Levy, an MIT professor and co-author of a new paper summarizing the study. Or, as the paper states, the findings are consistent with the idea that children "lack rich grammatical knowledge at the outset of language learning but rapidly begin to generalize on the basis of structural regularities in their input."

The paper, "The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children's Early Speech," appears in the latest issue of Psychological Science. The authors are Levy, a professor in MIT's Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences; Stephan Meylann of the University of California at Berkeley; Michael Frank of Stanford University; and Brandon Roy of Stanford and the MIT Media Lab.

Learning curve

Studying how children use terms such as "a dog" or "the dog" correctly can be a productive approach to language acquisition, since children use the articles "a" and "the" relatively early in their lives and tend to use them correctly. Again, though: Is that understanding of grammar innate or acquired?

Some previous studies have examined this specific question by using an "overlap score," that is, the proportion of nouns that children use with both "a" and "the," out of all the nouns they use. When children use both terms correctly, it indicates they understand the grammatical difference between indefinite and definite articles, as opposed to cases where they may (incorrectly) think only one or the other is assigned to a particular noun.

One potential drawback to this approach, however, is that the overlap score might change over time simply because a child might hear more article-noun pairings, without fully recognizing the grammatical distinction between articles.

By contrast, the current study builds a statistical model of language use that incorporates not only child language use but adult language use recorded around children, from a variety of sources. Some of these are publicly available copora of recordings of children and caregivers; others are records of individual children; and one source is the "Speechome" experiment conducted by Deb Roy of the MIT Media Lab, which features recordings of over 70 percent of his child's waking hours.

The Speechome data, as the paper notes, provides some of the strongest evidence yet that "children's syntactic productivity changes over development" -- that younger children learn grammar from hearing it, and do so at different rates during different phases of early childhood.

"I think the method starts to get us traction on the problem," Levy says. "We saw this as an opportunity both to use more comprehensive data and to develop new analytic techniques."

A work in progress

Still, as the authors note, a second conclusion of the paper is that more basic data about language development is needed. As the paper notes, much of the available information is not comprehensive enough, and thus "likely not sufficient to yield precise developmental conclusions."

And as Levy readily acknowledges, developing an airtight hypothesis about grammar acquisition is always likely to be a challenge.

"We're never going to have an absolute complete record of everything a child has ever heard," Levy says.

That makes it much harder to interpret the cognitive process leading to either correct or incorrect uses of, say, articles such as "a" and "the." After all, if a child uses the phrase "a bus" correctly, it still might only be because that child has heard the phrase before and likes the way it sounds, not because he or she grasped the underlying grammar.

"Those things are very hard to tease apart, but that's what we're trying to do," Levy says. "This is only really an initial step."
-end-
Additional background

Paper: The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children's Early Speech http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0956797616677753

ARCHIVE: Why children confuse simple words http://news.mit.edu/2016/why-children-confuse-simple-words

ARCHIVE: The rapid rise of human language http://news.mit.edu/2015/rapid-rise-human-language-0331

ARCHIVE: Decoding "noisy" language in daily life http://news.mit.edu/2013/decoding-noisy-language-0429

Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Related Language Articles:

Why the language-ready brain is so complex
In a review article published in Science, Peter Hagoort, professor of Cognitive Neuroscience at Radboud University and director of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, argues for a new model of language, involving the interaction of multiple brain networks.
Do as i say: Translating language into movement
Researchers at Carnegie Mellon University have developed a computer model that can translate text describing physical movements directly into simple computer-generated animations, a first step toward someday generating movies directly from scripts.
Learning language
When it comes to learning a language, the left side of the brain has traditionally been considered the hub of language processing.
Learning a second alphabet for a first language
A part of the brain that maps letters to sounds can acquire a second, visually distinct alphabet for the same language, according to a study of English speakers published in eNeuro.
Sign language reveals the hidden logical structure, and limitations, of spoken language
Sign languages can help reveal hidden aspects of the logical structure of spoken language, but they also highlight its limitations because speech lacks the rich iconic resources that sign language uses on top of its sophisticated grammar.
Lying in a foreign language is easier
It is not easy to tell when someone is lying.
American sign language and English language learners: New linguistic research supports the need for policy changes
A new study of the educational needs of students who are native users of American Sign Language (ASL) shows glaring disparities in their treatment by the U.S Department of Education.
The language of facial expressions
University of Miami Psychology Professor Daniel Messinger collaborated with researchers at Western University in Canada to show that our brains are pre-wired to perceive wrinkles around the eyes as conveying more intense and sincere emotions.
The universal language of hormones
Bioinformatics specialists from the University of W├╝rzburg have studied a specific class of hormones which is relevant for plants, bacteria and indirectly for humans, too.
Stretching language to its limit
A disregard for human traditions, the brutality of predation, sacrifice, and sexual desire are ingrained in languages across cultures.
More Language News and Language Current Events

Best Science Podcasts 2019

We have hand picked the best science podcasts for 2019. Sit back and enjoy new science podcasts updated daily from your favorite science news services and scientists.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Rethinking Anger
Anger is universal and complex: it can be quiet, festering, justified, vengeful, and destructive. This hour, TED speakers explore the many sides of anger, why we need it, and who's allowed to feel it. Guests include psychologists Ryan Martin and Russell Kolts, writer Soraya Chemaly, former talk radio host Lisa Fritsch, and business professor Dan Moshavi.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#538 Nobels and Astrophysics
This week we start with this year's physics Nobel Prize awarded to Jim Peebles, Michel Mayor, and Didier Queloz and finish with a discussion of the Nobel Prizes as a way to award and highlight important science. Are they still relevant? When science breakthroughs are built on the backs of hundreds -- and sometimes thousands -- of people's hard work, how do you pick just three to highlight? Join host Rachelle Saunders and astrophysicist, author, and science communicator Ethan Siegel for their chat about astrophysics and Nobel Prizes.