Duke Researchers Find Brain's Motor Center Keeps Time Too

April 29, 1996

BALTIMORE, Md. -- By measuring activity in the brain as reflected by blood flow, Duke researchers have demonstrated for the first time that the brain's motor control center also keeps track of time. Their experiments show that in both animals and people, the striatum, a portion of the brain once thought only to control movement, keeps track of timing short intervals, from seconds to minutes.

In addition to providing the first map of a neural circuit for an internal clock, the results have implications for Parkinson's disease patients, because the timing mechanism is located within the basal ganglia, which is damaged in people with Parkinson's disease. The findings also may help define the role of timing in learning and memory, said Dr. Warren Meck, associate professor of experimental psychology at Duke University.

Meck reported on the research Monday in a report prepared for the 1996 annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science.

"We believe timing is the foundation for learning and memory," Meck said in an interview. He suggests that defective timing mechanisms may underlie some learning disabilities and may contribute to dyslexia. Before these experiments, how the brain keeps track of time intervals in the seconds to minutes range was unknown.

"In the animal world, the ability to time short intervals is key to survival," Meck said. "Animals have to be able to determine, for example, if they are getting enough food to eat during a given time interval. They have to have a sense when the yield is no longer worth the effort, and they should move on."

People use this same internal clock to determine if they have enough time to cross a street before an oncoming car reaches them, says Meck.

"The master interval timers are short-order cooks," Meck said. "They have to keep track of multiple items cooking simultaneously, all of which will be done at different time intervals. They have to develop a sense of when the two-minute egg is done, versus the toast, versus the bacon. To do this, they rely on an internal clock and a memory of how long these tasks seemed to take last time."

To measure which parts of the brain are activated when a person needs to keep track of short time intervals, Meck and collaborator Dr. James MacFall, a Duke radiologist, used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a new application of clinical MR imaging, which measures the magnetic properties of water inside the body to create images of body organs non-invasively. The MR device measures increases in blood flow, and therefore activity in the brain, and translates that information into images.

Meck and graduate student Sean Hinton asked volunteers to estimate 11-second intervals, without counting, by squeezing a ball just before, or just after, the interval. The researchers measured which parts of the brain were activated while the volunteers were timing the interval. When they subtracted out data on brain activation due to the sensory and motor aspects of the task, they found that some of the most active parts of the brain were the frontal cortex and the striatum, a portion of the brain previously thought to be involved only with motor skills.

The fMRI data support Meck's previous experiments with rats, which he trained to press a lever after a specified time for a food reward. Once they had learned the correct time interval, the rats were given drugs that selectively kill neurons in the part of the basal ganglia called the substantia nigra. This area of the brain normally produces the neurotransmitter dopamine. It is this same area of the brain that is destroyed in Parkinson's disease.

Without functional dopamine-producing neurons in the basal ganglia, the rats could no longer time the duration they had learned previously, although their physical ability to do the task had not been impaired.

But when the rats were given L-dopa, a drug used to treat Parkinson's disease patients, their ability to estimate short time intervals was restored.

"These results suggest that dopamine-producing neurons innervating striatum are crucial to measuring short time intervals," said Meck. "When the striatum is damaged, the ability to learn and remember short time intervals is similarly damaged."

By selectively severing specific nerves in the brains of trained rats, Meck has localized the different parts of the brain that contribute to timing short intervals. The substantia nigra appears to function as a metronome, sending a steady stream of pulses to the striatum. This region, which is also part of the basal ganglia, appears to be a gatekeeper that turns on and off awareness of time intervals and feed that information to the frontal cortex, which stores the information in memory. The complete neural circuit is called a frontal-striatal loop.

"These studies demonstrate for the first time the importance of frontal-striatal loops in people timing short intervals," Meck said. "Now that we are beginning to understand how the brain processes short time intervals, we can explore how timing is integrated with learning and memory. We now have the tools to begin assessing these questions."
-end-


Duke University

Related Memory Articles from Brightsurf:

Memory of the Venus flytrap
In a study to be published in Nature Plants, a graduate student Mr.

Memory protein
When UC Santa Barbara materials scientist Omar Saleh and graduate student Ian Morgan sought to understand the mechanical behaviors of disordered proteins in the lab, they expected that after being stretched, one particular model protein would snap back instantaneously, like a rubber band.

Previously claimed memory boosting font 'Sans Forgetica' does not actually boost memory
It was previously claimed that the font Sans Forgetica could enhance people's memory for information, however researchers from the University of Warwick and the University of Waikato, New Zealand, have found after carrying out numerous experiments that the font does not enhance memory.

Memory boost with just one look
HRL Laboratories, LLC, researchers have published results showing that targeted transcranial electrical stimulation during slow-wave sleep can improve metamemories of specific episodes by 20% after only one viewing of the episode, compared to controls.

VR is not suited to visual memory?!
Toyohashi university of technology researcher and a research team at Tokyo Denki University have found that virtual reality (VR) may interfere with visual memory.

The genetic signature of memory
Despite their importance in memory, the human cortex and subcortex display a distinct collection of 'gene signatures.' The work recently published in eNeuro increases our understanding of how the brain creates memories and identifies potential genes for further investigation.

How long does memory last? For shape memory alloys, the longer the better
Scientists captured live action details of the phase transitions of shape memory alloys, giving them a better idea how to improve their properties for applications.

A NEAT discovery about memory
UAB researchers say over expression of NEAT1, an noncoding RNA, appears to diminish the ability of older brains to form memories.

Molecular memory can be used to increase the memory capacity of hard disks
Researchers at the University of Jyväskylä have taken part in an international British-Finnish-Chinese collaboration where the first molecule capable of remembering the direction of a magnetic above liquid nitrogen temperatures has been prepared and characterized.

Memory transferred between snails
Memories can be transferred between organisms by extracting ribonucleic acid (RNA) from a trained animal and injecting it into an untrained animal, as demonstrated in a study of sea snails published in eNeuro.

Read More: Memory News and Memory Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.