Nav: Home

'Doing science,' rather than 'being scientists,' more encouraging to those underrepresented in the field

May 13, 2019

Over the course of a school year, elementary school children lose confidence that they can "be scientists," but remain more confident that they can "do science," finds a new psychology study by researchers at New York University and Princeton University.

The work, which appears in the journal Developmental Science, also found that children think more adults in their community can "do science" than "are scientists," suggesting that children have more inclusive views of who can do science, even while they might hold stereotypes about who can be a scientist.

"Action-focused language--instead of identity-focused encouragement--leads children to hold more inclusive beliefs about who can succeed in science and bolsters science efficacy and interest, particularly among children from ethnic minority groups that are underrepresented in science," explains Marjorie Rhodes, an associate professor in NYU's Department of Psychology and the senior author of the study.

The results are consistent with those reported earlier this year by Rhodes' research team. In a February study that appeared in the journal Psychological Science, the researchers found that asking young girls to "do science" leads them to show greater persistence in subsequent science activities than does asking them to "be scientists." The samples in these previous studies were primarily white, however, and the researchers hypothesized that the benefits of action-focused language would extend more broadly (to children of both genders) in more racially, ethnically, and economically diverse samples.

In the new Developmental Science study, which was led by Ryan Lei, an NYU post-doctoral research fellow, and also included Sarah-Jane Leslie, a professor of philosophy and dean of the Graduate School at Princeton University, and Emily Green, an NYU research scientist, the researchers studied more than 300 elementary-school children in Brooklyn and the Bronx over the course of the school year. Children in the study were primarily Hispanic, but reflected the racial diversity of their surrounding communities and were roughly evenly split between boys and girls.

"Studying a more diverse population is crucial if we want to understand and ensure efforts to improve science engagement work for everyone," says Lei. "That we see similar effects across children of different backgrounds in these communities suggests that using action-focused language could be a promising strategy to help a large number of children stay engaged in science."

The researchers measured children's interest and self-efficacy in science three times across the course of a school year (once in the fall, once in the middle of the school year, and once in the late spring). Half of the children were asked at each point how interested they were in "being a scientist" and how good they thought they were at "being a scientist," whereas the other half were asked how interested in and good they thought they were at "doing science." The questions may be viewed at https://osf.io/56fg9/.

The results showed that, over the course of the school year, children's confidence and interest in "being a scientist" declined. By contrast, they maintained confidence and interest in their capacities to "do science," thereby showing that persistent curiosity in science is linked to messages about actions and not identity.

The researchers also examined what might underlie these effects by asking how children see the group of people who "do science" or "are scientists." To measure this, Rhodes and her colleagues asked the children to think of all the parents of the kids at their school and to judge how many of those parents either "were scientists" or "did science." Results showed that children thought more adults in their community "did science" than "were scientists," and these beliefs partially accounted for the effects of language on their own interest and efficacy.

"These finding suggest that using identity-focused language with children, such as asking them to 'be a scientist,' can, in fact, backfire whenever children have reason to question if they are really a member of the group," explains Rhodes. "Such reasons to question can come from social stereotypes--such as a belief that few people from a child's community can grow up to be a scientist."

"This research indicates that a subtle change in how we talk about science with children--using more action-oriented language--can potentially guard against declines in children's science interest and self-efficacy," she adds.
-end-
Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (R01HD087672).

DOI: 10.1111/desc.12837

New York University

Related Language Articles:

Chinese to rise as a global language
With the continuing rise of China as a global economic and trading power, there is no barrier to prevent Chinese from becoming a global language like English, according to Flinders University academic Dr Jeffrey Gil.
'She' goes missing from presidential language
MIT researchers have found that although a significant percentage of the American public believed the winner of the November 2016 presidential election would be a woman, people rarely used the pronoun 'she' when referring to the next president before the election.
How does language emerge?
How did the almost 6000 languages of the world come into being?
New research quantifies how much speakers' first language affects learning a new language
Linguistic research suggests that accents are strongly shaped by the speaker's first language they learned growing up.
Why the language-ready brain is so complex
In a review article published in Science, Peter Hagoort, professor of Cognitive Neuroscience at Radboud University and director of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, argues for a new model of language, involving the interaction of multiple brain networks.
Do as i say: Translating language into movement
Researchers at Carnegie Mellon University have developed a computer model that can translate text describing physical movements directly into simple computer-generated animations, a first step toward someday generating movies directly from scripts.
Learning language
When it comes to learning a language, the left side of the brain has traditionally been considered the hub of language processing.
Learning a second alphabet for a first language
A part of the brain that maps letters to sounds can acquire a second, visually distinct alphabet for the same language, according to a study of English speakers published in eNeuro.
Sign language reveals the hidden logical structure, and limitations, of spoken language
Sign languages can help reveal hidden aspects of the logical structure of spoken language, but they also highlight its limitations because speech lacks the rich iconic resources that sign language uses on top of its sophisticated grammar.
Lying in a foreign language is easier
It is not easy to tell when someone is lying.
More Language News and Language Current Events

Trending Science News

Current Coronavirus (COVID-19) News

Top Science Podcasts

We have hand picked the top science podcasts of 2020.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Listen Again: Meditations on Loneliness
Original broadcast date: April 24, 2020. We're a social species now living in isolation. But loneliness was a problem well before this era of social distancing. This hour, TED speakers explore how we can live and make peace with loneliness. Guests on the show include author and illustrator Jonny Sun, psychologist Susan Pinker, architect Grace Kim, and writer Suleika Jaouad.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#565 The Great Wide Indoors
We're all spending a bit more time indoors this summer than we probably figured. But did you ever stop to think about why the places we live and work as designed the way they are? And how they could be designed better? We're talking with Emily Anthes about her new book "The Great Indoors: The Surprising Science of how Buildings Shape our Behavior, Health and Happiness".
Now Playing: Radiolab

The Third. A TED Talk.
Jad gives a TED talk about his life as a journalist and how Radiolab has evolved over the years. Here's how TED described it:How do you end a story? Host of Radiolab Jad Abumrad tells how his search for an answer led him home to the mountains of Tennessee, where he met an unexpected teacher: Dolly Parton.Jad Nicholas Abumrad is a Lebanese-American radio host, composer and producer. He is the founder of the syndicated public radio program Radiolab, which is broadcast on over 600 radio stations nationwide and is downloaded more than 120 million times a year as a podcast. He also created More Perfect, a podcast that tells the stories behind the Supreme Court's most famous decisions. And most recently, Dolly Parton's America, a nine-episode podcast exploring the life and times of the iconic country music star. Abumrad has received three Peabody Awards and was named a MacArthur Fellow in 2011.