Echolocation: Making the best of sparse information

May 20, 2019

New findings reported by Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitaet (LMU) in Munich researchers challenge a generally accepted model of echolocation in bats. They demonstrate that bats require far less spatial information than previously thought to navigate effectively.

Bats find their way in the dark by emitting ultrasonic signals and analyzing the echoes generated when these signals impinge on, and are reflected by, objects in their environment. Experiments carried out by LMU researchers now refute a central postulate of the conventional model of echolocation. "Up to now, it has been assumed that the echoes provide a kind of acoustic image of the environment, which allows bats not only to detect objects but also to perceive the gaps between them. This notion turns out to be wrong. Our experiments show that bats have very little ability to spatially resolve objects. Their navigational system makes do with a surprisingly small amount of spatial information, and its mode of operation differs fundamentally from our conventional picture of echolocation," says Lutz Wiegrebe, a professor in the Neurobiology Division. The new findings appear in the latest issue of the journal Current Biology.

In their study, Wiegrebe and colleagues used bats that had been trained to emit ultrasonic signals when they detected the acoustic reflections of objects in their environment. In the experimental set-up, the bats emitted signals which were reflected from a target placed directly in front of them, but were also exposed to reflections originating from sources to the left and right of the target of interest. The latter therefore served as cluttering targets which were displaced from the target of interest by angular distances that were varied from experiment to experiment. "The bats were extremely irritated by the virtual reflections, and were able to detect the target of interest only when the virtual targets were placed relatively far away from it."

This paradigm used to measure the spatial resolution of the bat echolocation system is analogous to that used to measure the spatial resolution of the visual system. In the mammalian retina, the anatomical organization of the photoreceptor cells itself provides the basis for high spatial resolution. "But echolocation works in an entirely different way," Wiegrebe explains. "The sensory cells in the inner ear do not encode axes in space, they encode time and frequency. Spatial information must be secondarily derived from these inputs by neuronal processing. Our experiments demonstrate that the spatial resolution of the bat echolocation system is approximately three orders of magnitude worse than that of the human visual system. In other words, bats navigating by echolocation receive an extremely fuzzy picture of their environment. A visually simple task like 'how many fingers am I holding up' is extremely difficult to solve with biosonar. Nevertheless, bats can navigate very effectively." The authors of the new study believe that the animals compensate for the deficit in spatial resolution by constantly updating the incoming information relating to the distance and direction of objects, and concentrating on determining the relative positions of the objects that are closest to them.

The results may well have implications for manmade navigation systems. "The navigational systems used in e.g. autonomous vehicles use a camera to capture a continuous stream of information. It is conceivable that much of this information is redundant. Bats use a much simpler scheme that does not depend on the acquisition of a 3-D image of their environment, such as that provided by the visual system. Yet they can navigate quite effectively."

In a paper recently published in the journal iScience, Leonie Baier (a member of Lutz Wiegrebe's research group) and Dr. Holger Goerlitz of the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology showed that some bat species are preferentially sensitive to surface-wave patterns with high spatial frequencies, which allows them to gauge depth, i.e. differentiate between foreground and background. This ability enables them to detect an insect on the surface of a pond, for instance. "Our work has now shown that bats cannot resolve this information spatially," says Wiegrebe. If the water surface is covered with vegetation, a foraging bat can no longer localize the insect.
-end-


Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München

Related Bats Articles from Brightsurf:

These masked singers are bats
Bats wear face masks, too. Bat researchers got lucky, observing wrinkle-faced bats in a lek, and copulating, for the first time.

Why do bats fly into walls?
Bats sometimes collide with large walls even though they detect these walls with their sonar system.

Vampire bats social distance when they get sick
A new paper in Behavioral Ecology finds that wild vampire bats that are sick spend less time near others from their community, which slows how quickly a disease will spread.

Why doesn't Ebola cause disease in bats, as it does in people?
A new study by researchers from The University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston uncovered new information on why the Ebola virus can live within bats without causing them harm, while the same virus wreaks deadly havoc to people.

The genetic basis of bats' superpowers revealed
First six reference-quality bat genomes released and analysed

Bats offer clues to treating COVID-19
Bats carry many viruses, including COVID-19, without becoming ill. Biologists at the University of Rochester are studying the immune system of bats to find potential ways to ''mimic'' that system in humans.

A new social role for echolocation in bats that hunt together
To find prey in the dark, bats use echolocation. Some species, like Molossus molossus, may also search within hearing distance of their echolocating group members, sharing information about where food patches are located.

Coronaviruses and bats have been evolving together for millions of years
Scientists compared the different kinds of coronaviruses living in 36 bat species from the western Indian Ocean and nearby areas of Africa.

Bats depend on conspecifics when hunting above farmland
Common noctules -- one of the largest bat species native to Germany -- are searching for their fellows during their hunt for insects above farmland.

Tiny insects become 'visible' to bats when they swarm
Small insects that would normally be undetectable to bats using echolocation suddenly become detectable when they occur in large swarms.

Read More: Bats News and Bats Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.