Gene found responsible for social amnesia

June 18, 2000

What if you could find your way home, but couldn't recognize anyone when you got there?

Reporting in the July issue of Nature Genetics, scientists at the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience at the Yerkes Primate Center have discovered in mouse studies that the oxytocin gene is necessary for forming social memories -- allowing you to recognize an individual you've seen before.

The gene has no apparent influence on spatial or other types of memory. The study demonstrates that social memory has a neural basis distinct from other forms of memory, and someday could provide a therapeutic target for a variety of psychiatric illnesses.

The study's author, Jim Winslow, Ph.D., associate research professor of psychobiology at Yerkes, says the ability to recognize those we have met before is the first step in the process of developing an affiliative relationship. "Without this fundamental ability, even your own mother would remain a stranger," he says. It is thought that such a defect could play a role in autism and schizophrenia, which are characterized by a sense of social disconnection and isolation.

The gene in question codes for the brain hormone oxytocin (OT), which is present in all mammals, including humans. This neuropeptide has long been associated in many species with a range of social behaviors, including parental care (such as nursing and parturition), pair bonding and mate-guarding. In humans, oxytocin peaks during ejaculation. To date, there have been no studies in humans on the role of OT in pair bonding, though studies in monkeys show that increased transmission of the peptide does increase social interaction.

Dr. Winslow's team made its discovery with the help of a transgenic or "knockout" mouse, engineered to lack the gene for oxytocin. In the study, knockout mice were compared with normal mice in tests for social and non-social memory.

Rodents depend largely on olfactory cues to "know" the world around them. They have two olfactory systems: a primary one for food and other non-social scents that help them navigate their environment, and an ancillary system to detect social scents in the form of pheromones, the odors released to provide social information such as sexual readiness or territorial prerogative. A normal male mouse will follow its nose to vigorously investigate a stranger until the stranger becomes familiar, usually within about five minutes. At that point the investigation tapers off significantly and other types of social behavior -- such as mounting or fighting or just being friends -- commence. When a new female is introduced, interest and olfactory investigation return to previous levels.

By contrast, Dr. Winslow's group found that the knockout mice, which lack the gene for oxytocin, failed to recognize mice with whom they had previously interacted. They continued their robust olfactory investigation as though they had never seen the mice before, although they should have been familiar. "The knockout mice clearly showed a social memory deficit," says Dr. Winslow.

To be sure the problem didn't lie with generally impaired olfactory functioning, Winslow's team tested the animals' all-important sense of smell with various food-foraging tasks. The normal and transgenic mice both were able to find buried food as quickly as food placed clearly in view -- suggesting they could find the hidden food through their sense of smell. Both rapidly became "familiar" with scented foods, such as lemon and chocolate, and recognized when a scent was changed, showing that olfactory function was not measurably influenced by the lack of oxytocin.

In addition, the scientists tested the spatial memory of the mice using a water maze task, to see if other forms of memory were also impaired due to the lack of oxytocin. Transgenic mice were every bit as adept at finding their way around the maze as were normal mice, demonstrating that spatial memory was intact.

Dr. Winslow was successful in restoring social memory formation by treating the knockout mice with oxytocin. In these mice, social amnesia disappeared. Like the normal mice, they again showed the characteristic decline in time spent investigating familiar females, and recovered interest when a new female entered the cage.

Virtually all forms of psychopathology, which include some of man's most debilitating clinical disorders, are characterized by abnormal social attachments. Yet very little is known about the normal process of bond formation. This work helps lay the groundwork for defining the neural basis of attachment and identifying potential pharmacological targets in the brain for future therapies.
-end-
This study was funded by grants from Emory University, the National Institutes of Health and the National Alliance for Autism Research. Yerkes Primate Research Center is the oldest scientific institution dedicated to primate research. Its programs cover a wide range of biomedical and behavioral sciences.




Emory University Health Sciences Center

Related Memory Articles from Brightsurf:

Memory of the Venus flytrap
In a study to be published in Nature Plants, a graduate student Mr.

Memory protein
When UC Santa Barbara materials scientist Omar Saleh and graduate student Ian Morgan sought to understand the mechanical behaviors of disordered proteins in the lab, they expected that after being stretched, one particular model protein would snap back instantaneously, like a rubber band.

Previously claimed memory boosting font 'Sans Forgetica' does not actually boost memory
It was previously claimed that the font Sans Forgetica could enhance people's memory for information, however researchers from the University of Warwick and the University of Waikato, New Zealand, have found after carrying out numerous experiments that the font does not enhance memory.

Memory boost with just one look
HRL Laboratories, LLC, researchers have published results showing that targeted transcranial electrical stimulation during slow-wave sleep can improve metamemories of specific episodes by 20% after only one viewing of the episode, compared to controls.

VR is not suited to visual memory?!
Toyohashi university of technology researcher and a research team at Tokyo Denki University have found that virtual reality (VR) may interfere with visual memory.

The genetic signature of memory
Despite their importance in memory, the human cortex and subcortex display a distinct collection of 'gene signatures.' The work recently published in eNeuro increases our understanding of how the brain creates memories and identifies potential genes for further investigation.

How long does memory last? For shape memory alloys, the longer the better
Scientists captured live action details of the phase transitions of shape memory alloys, giving them a better idea how to improve their properties for applications.

A NEAT discovery about memory
UAB researchers say over expression of NEAT1, an noncoding RNA, appears to diminish the ability of older brains to form memories.

Molecular memory can be used to increase the memory capacity of hard disks
Researchers at the University of Jyväskylä have taken part in an international British-Finnish-Chinese collaboration where the first molecule capable of remembering the direction of a magnetic above liquid nitrogen temperatures has been prepared and characterized.

Memory transferred between snails
Memories can be transferred between organisms by extracting ribonucleic acid (RNA) from a trained animal and injecting it into an untrained animal, as demonstrated in a study of sea snails published in eNeuro.

Read More: Memory News and Memory Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.