How the visual system constructs moving objects: 1 by 1

August 12, 2011

Although our eyes record the word as millions of pixels, "the visual system is fantastic at giving us a world that looks like objects, not pixels," says Northwestern University psychologist Steven L. Franconeri. It does this by grouping areas of the world with similar characteristics, such as color, shape, or motion.

The process is so seamless that we feel we're taking it all in simultaneously. But this, says a new study by Franconeri and his colleague Brian R. Levinthal, is "an illusion." Instead, they say, that for some types of grouping, the visual system is limited by its ability to perceive only one group at a time. The findings were just published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

How does this grouping work? Say you're looking at a crowded street, with cars going every which way. Your view of each individual car is partially blocked, so that you actually see multiple "pieces" of each. Yet because those pieces move with the same direction of motion, grouping by "common fate" helps you perceive whole cars. You might feel that this grouping is happening for all the cars at once--but Levinthal and Franconeri suggest otherwise.

To demonstrate this point, the authors performed two experiments, simplifying the "car pieces" into pairs of moving dots. In the first experiment, participants had to find the pairs that were vertically arranged among pairs that were horizontally arranged. In the second, instead of finding a group with a specific shape, they were asked to find a group among non-groups--like whole cars among scattered pieces. The researchers conjectured that if it's possible to perceive all groups at once, then finding one with a specific shape should be easy. If the visual system can construct only one group at a time, then this task should become progressively harder as more groups are added to the screen.

In both tasks, "people were surprisingly slow," says Franconeri. As participants were asked to make decisions involving more groups, they took more and more time. They were limited by their one-at-a-time visual systems, which, he says, "needed to flip through the groups, at a rate of about 10 per second."

The research, says Franconeri, offers practical information for people creating effective complex graphic systems such as charts and graphs, and even more complex visualizations, such as genomic sequences.

It also enhances science's understanding of ordinary vision. "The visual system fools us into thinking that we process everything in rich detail, when in many cases we are processing only the most relevant pieces of the world," says Franconeri. The new study adds another insight: "You think you're seeing all these moving groups. But in fact you're seeing only the group you are 'looking at' with your mind's eye"--one by one by one.
-end-
For more information about this study, please contact: Steven L. Franconeri at franconeri@northwestern.edu.

The APS journal Psychological Science is the highest ranked empirical journal in psychology. For a copy of the article "Common fate grouping as feature selection" and access to other Psychological Science research findings, please contact Divya Menon at 202-293-9300 or dmenon@psychologicalscience.org.

Association for Psychological Science

Related Visual System Articles from Brightsurf:

Visual working memory is hierarchically structured
Researchers from HSE University and the University of California San Diego, Igor Utochkin and Timothy Brady, have found new evidence of hierarchical encoding of images in visual working memory.

The mystery of visual stability
We move our eyes several times per second. These fast eye movements, called saccades, create large image shifts on the retina -- making our visual system work hard to maintain a stable perceptual world.

Why visual perception is a decision process
A popular theory in neuroscience called predictive coding proposes that the brain produces all the time expectations that are compared with incoming information.

Visual impairment among women and dementia risk
Whether visual impairment is a risk factor for dementia was the focus of this observational study that included 1,000 older women who are participants in the Women's Health Initiative studies.

VR is not suited to visual memory?!
Toyohashi university of technology researcher and a research team at Tokyo Denki University have found that virtual reality (VR) may interfere with visual memory.

Dartmouth study finds conscious visual perception occurs outside the visual system
A Dartmouth study finds that the conscious perception of visual location occurs in the frontal lobes of the brain, rather than in the visual system in the back of the brain.

Learning to read boosts the visual brain
How does learning to read change our brain? Does reading take up brain space dedicated to seeing objects such as faces, tools or houses?

How brain rhythms organize our visual perception
Imagine that you are watching a crowded hang-gliding competition, keeping track of a red and orange glider's skillful movements.

Seeing it both ways: Visual perspective in memory
Think of a memory from your childhood. Are you seeing the memory through your own eyes, or can you see yourself, while viewing that child as if you were an observer?

Using visual imagery to find your true passions
You may think you know what you like -- how to spend your time or what profession to pursue.

Read More: Visual System News and Visual System Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.