Nav: Home

Memory protein

August 25, 2020

When UC Santa Barbara materials scientist Omar Saleh and graduate student Ian Morgan sought to understand the mechanical behaviors of disordered proteins in the lab, they expected that after being stretched, one particular model protein would snap back instantaneously, like a rubber band.

Instead, this disordered protein relaxed slowly, taking tens of minutes to relax into its original shape -- a behavior that defied expectations, and hinted at an inner structure that was long thought to exist, but has been difficult to prove.

"The speed of relaxation is important because it gives us some insight into the structural organization of the protein," said Morgan, the lead author in a paper published in Physical Review Letters. "This is important because the structural organization of a protein is usually related to its biological function."

While a protein with fixed 'folds' -- a well-defined three-dimensional structure -- is associated with its function, disordered proteins, with their unstable structures, derive their functions from their dynamics.

"More than 40% of human proteins are at least partially unfolded, and they are often linked to critical biological processes as well as debilitating diseases," Morgan said.

The slow relaxation is in fact a behavior typically reserved for folded proteins.

"In the 1980s it was discovered that folded proteins exhibit slow relaxations," Morgan said, in a behavior typical of glasses -- a class of materials that are neither truly liquid nor crystalline solid states, but can exhibit characteristics of either state.

"We have been studying folded proteins for a long time and have developed a lot of good tools for them, so it was quickly figured out that the slow relaxations could be explained by a mechanism by which 'frustrated' molecules trying to fit themselves in a small space," Morgan said -- a mechanism called "jamming." "This explanation helped us better understand the structure of folded proteins and explain glassy behavior in a lot of other systems."

However, the protein, which the researchers were trying to stretch by means of a device known as a magnetic tweezer, was a disordered protein. By definition, it wasn't trying to pack many molecules into a small space, so it shouldn't run into the jamming problem, Saleh said.

"So, when we observed slow relaxations, it either meant our definition of the protein was wrong or there had to be another mechanism," Morgan said.

Furthermore, by allowing the stretched protein to relax but stretching it again with less force before it had a chance to fully relax, the researchers found that the protein "remembered" its previous stretching -- initially lengthening, as expected with more force, but eventually slowly relaxing again lengthening as expected with less force, but then slowly relaxing over time. Conceptually, Morgan explained, the longer the protein is stretched the longer it takes to relax, hence it "remembers" how long it was pulled.

To explain these unexpected, glassy behaviors, the researchers drew inspiration from some rather mundane objects: crumpled paper and memory foam. Both structurally disordered systems, they exhibit a similar slow, logarithmic relaxation after being subjected to forces, and particularly in the case of the foam, a "memory" effect.

For the researchers the behaviors suggested that like memory foam and crumpled paper, the internal structure of the protein was not one of a single, fixed unit, but one of several, independent substructures of a range of strengths between strong and weak that respond to a range of forces exerted on the material along different lengths of time. For instance, strong structures may withstand a certain amount of strain before being pulled apart and be the first to relax, whereas weak structures will stretch with smaller forces and take longer to relax.

Based on this notion of multiple substructures and confirmed with experimental data, the researchers determined that the protein's logarithmic relaxation rate is inversely proportional to the stretching force.

"The stronger the stretching force applied to the disordered protein, the more the protein relaxed in the same amount of time," Saleh explained.

"Mechanical disordered systems with similar structural arrangements tend to be remarkably durable," Morgan said. "They also have different mechanical properties depending on how much you pull and compress them. This makes them very adaptable, depending on the magnitude and frequency of the force." Understanding the structure behind this ability to adapt could open the door to future dynamic materials, that, Morgan said, "just like your brain, helps them filter out unimportant information and makes them more efficient at storing repeated stimuli."
-end-
Research on this study was conducted also by Ram Avinery, Gil Rahamim and Roy Beck at Tel Aviv University.

This work was made possible by a joint award from the National Science Foundation and Israel Science Foundation via the U.S.-Israel Binational Science Foundation.

University of California - Santa Barbara

Related Memory Articles:

Memory of the Venus flytrap
In a study to be published in Nature Plants, a graduate student Mr.
Memory protein
When UC Santa Barbara materials scientist Omar Saleh and graduate student Ian Morgan sought to understand the mechanical behaviors of disordered proteins in the lab, they expected that after being stretched, one particular model protein would snap back instantaneously, like a rubber band.
Previously claimed memory boosting font 'Sans Forgetica' does not actually boost memory
It was previously claimed that the font Sans Forgetica could enhance people's memory for information, however researchers from the University of Warwick and the University of Waikato, New Zealand, have found after carrying out numerous experiments that the font does not enhance memory.
Memory boost with just one look
HRL Laboratories, LLC, researchers have published results showing that targeted transcranial electrical stimulation during slow-wave sleep can improve metamemories of specific episodes by 20% after only one viewing of the episode, compared to controls.
VR is not suited to visual memory?!
Toyohashi university of technology researcher and a research team at Tokyo Denki University have found that virtual reality (VR) may interfere with visual memory.
The genetic signature of memory
Despite their importance in memory, the human cortex and subcortex display a distinct collection of 'gene signatures.' The work recently published in eNeuro increases our understanding of how the brain creates memories and identifies potential genes for further investigation.
How long does memory last? For shape memory alloys, the longer the better
Scientists captured live action details of the phase transitions of shape memory alloys, giving them a better idea how to improve their properties for applications.
A NEAT discovery about memory
UAB researchers say over expression of NEAT1, an noncoding RNA, appears to diminish the ability of older brains to form memories.
Molecular memory can be used to increase the memory capacity of hard disks
Researchers at the University of Jyväskylä have taken part in an international British-Finnish-Chinese collaboration where the first molecule capable of remembering the direction of a magnetic above liquid nitrogen temperatures has been prepared and characterized.
Memory transferred between snails
Memories can be transferred between organisms by extracting ribonucleic acid (RNA) from a trained animal and injecting it into an untrained animal, as demonstrated in a study of sea snails published in eNeuro.
More Memory News and Memory Current Events

Trending Science News

Current Coronavirus (COVID-19) News

Top Science Podcasts

We have hand picked the top science podcasts of 2020.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Sound And Silence
Sound surrounds us, from cacophony even to silence. But depending on how we hear, the world can be a different auditory experience for each of us. This hour, TED speakers explore the science of sound. Guests on the show include NPR All Things Considered host Mary Louise Kelly, neuroscientist Jim Hudspeth, writer Rebecca Knill, and sound designer Dallas Taylor.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#576 Science Communication in Creative Places
When you think of science communication, you might think of TED talks or museum talks or video talks, or... people giving lectures. It's a lot of people talking. But there's more to sci comm than that. This week host Bethany Brookshire talks to three people who have looked at science communication in places you might not expect it. We'll speak with Mauna Dasari, a graduate student at Notre Dame, about making mammals into a March Madness match. We'll talk with Sarah Garner, director of the Pathologists Assistant Program at Tulane University School of Medicine, who takes pathology instruction out of...
Now Playing: Radiolab

Kittens Kick The Giggly Blue Robot All Summer
With the recent passing of Ruth Bader Ginsburg, there's been a lot of debate about how much power the Supreme Court should really have. We think of the Supreme Court justices as all-powerful beings, issuing momentous rulings from on high. But they haven't always been so, you know, supreme. On this episode, we go all the way back to the case that, in a lot of ways, started it all.  Support Radiolab by becoming a member today at Radiolab.org/donate.