Nav: Home

Low lead levels in children negatively affect test scores

October 05, 2016

PROVIDENCE, R.I. [Brown University] --A decrease in the average level of lead in a preschooler's blood reduces the probability of that child being substantially below proficient in reading by the third grade, a new National Bureau of Economics Research working paper reports. And because poor and minority children are more likely to be exposed to lead, the study suggests that lead poisoning may be one of the causes of continuing gaps in test scores between children from different socioeconomic groups.

"This study underscores the importance of looking at factors outside the educational setting to help explain persistent gaps in test scores," said Anna Aizer, professor of economics at Brown University.

Aizer co-authored the study, "Do Low Levels of Blood Lead Reduce Children's Future Test Scores?" with economist Janet Currie of Princeton University and Brown colleagues Dr. Peter Simon and Dr. Patrick Vivier.

Rhode Island's lead abatement and testing programs

The study looked at eight cohorts of Rhode Island children born between 1997 and 2005, a timeframe whose beginning coincides with two statewide remediation programs. One mandated that landlords in buildings where elevated levels of lead had been found remediate the lead, with the aid of state loans, or face prosecution. The other required all landlords to mitigate lead hazards in the homes they rented regardless of whether an elevated lead level had been reported.

Although lead was outlawed as a paint additive in 1978, much of Rhode Island's housing stock predates that ruling. According to the study, U.S. Census data reveals that 81 percent of the homes in Providence County were built prior to 1978 and 49 percent before World War II.

Minority and low-income children were disproportionately exposed to deteriorating lead-based paint, the authors wrote, because of "residential segregation of the poor, and especially of African Americans, in the four core urban areas of the state located within Providence County."

Rhode Island, the authors note, "had a particularly aggressive program of testing for lead, and 80 percent of all three-year-old children in the state have at least one blood lead level measurement." Most Rhode Island children have repeated tests for lead, according to the authors, so they were able to "match information on preschool blood lead levels from the R.I. Department of Health with the child's test scores from the R.I. Department of Education in order to examine the effects of preschool blood lead levels on third grade test scores."

Measuring lead in blood

Lead, which the authors note has been known to be toxic to the human body since Roman times, affects many of the body's systems, including the renal, endocrine and cardiovascular, "but the nervous system appears to be the most sensitive target," the authors wrote.

Blood lead levels (BLLs) are measured by micrograms of lead per deciliter. "The mean BLL is 3.1, which is well below the Center for Disease Control's threshold for medical intervention of 5 micrograms per deciliter," according to the study.

At the outset of the study, African American children had a mean BLL 5.77, while Hispanic children had a mean BLL of 4.91. By 2005, those concentrations had declined to 2.95 and 2.52 respectively, a decrease of 42 percent.

"This is a very rapid decline, which we attribute in part to the introduction of the lead-safe certificate program," Aizer and her colleagues wrote, noting that the most groups saw a dramatic increase in the percentage of children who lived in a home with a certificate in place at the time of the birth.

"In the 1997 birth cohort only two percent of sample African American children lived in a home that had been certified lead-safe at the time of their birth; by 2005, 12 percent of sample African American children lived in a home that had been certified as lead-safe from the time of their birth," the authors wrote.

Rising test scores in reading

The authors found that by third grade, test scores rose for all children in the study, regardless of demographic group, while the fraction scoring substantially below proficient declined. The greatest gains, the researchers found, were made by the children who were most affected by the certificate program.

The authors found different effects among different economic groups, including that "among children who never participated in the free lunch program, reading test scores rose by 5.9 percent and the percent who were not proficient in reading fell from six to four percent."

On the other hand, among children "who were always eligible for the free lunch program, reading test scores rose by 11.2 percent and the percent who were substantially below proficient in reading fell from 27 to 18 percent, a remarkable improvement."

The authors wrote that they recognized that positive parenting practices or educational policies that targeted poor and minority children could cause gains in reading scores. The researchers also acknowledge that children with higher lead levels "are more likely to come from minority groups, to be poor, to live in single parent homes, and to have less educated mothers. Such a bias would tend to cause researchers to overstate the dangers of lead."

To address these multiple concerns, the authors wrote, "we control for the average reading test scores of all the other students in a child's school and grade. This control captures potential changes in schools (policies, resources, student composition, etc.) that might be relevant to the index child's test scores." The researchers also discounted the additional reduction in blood lead levels in cases where it seemed as though parents were making a special effort to take advantage of the lead certificate program.

The negative effects of even historically low levels of lead

The researchers note that even though blood lead levels are at historically low levels, the negative impact on academic performance is significant.

Aizer, Currie, Simon and Vivier found a one point decline in reading scores for each one unit increase in the mean of a child's blood lead levels, as well as a 3.1 percentage point increase in the probability of being "substantially below proficient" in reading.

According to the study, estimates on blood lead levels' impact on math scores "are imprecisely estimated, though still suggestive of negative effects of lead."

The group's results suggest that Rhode Island's lead-safe certificate program, whose annual cost, they estimate, is about $500,000, caused lead levels to fall faster among disadvantaged children than among other children, and accounted for a third of the gain in reading scores among African American children and a fifth of the gain among Hispanic children over this period.
-end-


Brown University

Related Reading Articles:

'Reading' with aphasia is easier than 'running'
Neurolinguists from HSE University have confirmed experimentally that for people with aphasia, it is easier to retrieve verbs describing situations with several participants (such as 'someone is doing something'), although such verbs give rise to more grammar difficulties.
Hearing through lip-reading
Brain activity synchronizes with sound waves, even without audible sound, through lip-reading, according to new research published in JNeurosci.
Here's how you help kids crack the reading code
Some children learn to read early. Others need more time.
Cerebral reperfusion of reading network predicts recovery of reading ability after stroke
'Our findings support the utility of cerebral perfusion as a biomarker for recovery after stroke,' said Dr.
A lack of background knowledge can hinder reading comprehension
The purpose of going to school is to learn, but students may find certain topics difficult to understand if they don't have the necessary background knowledge.
A map of the brain can tell what you're reading
UC Berkeley neuroscientists have created interactive maps that can predict where different categories of words activate the brain.
Questions during shared book reading with preschoolers need to be more challenging
When it comes to challenging young minds to grow language, asking how and why during shared book reading to preschoolers can be more beneficial, according to new research at The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston (UTHealth).
Intellectual curiosity and confidence help children take on math and reading
Children's personalities may influence how they perform in math and reading, according to a study by psychology researchers at The University of Texas at Austin.
Reading between the lines: Are we as savvy as we'd like to think when it comes to reviews?
New research suggests we are willing to blindly trust hotel reviews when they conform to our preconceived ideas.
Kids connect with robot reading partners
Researchers at the University of Wisconsin-Madison have built a robot, named Minnie, to serve as a reading buddy to middle school kids, and Minnie's new friends grew more excited about books and more attached to the robot over two weeks of reading together.
More Reading News and Reading Current Events

Trending Science News

Current Coronavirus (COVID-19) News

Top Science Podcasts

We have hand picked the top science podcasts of 2020.
Now Playing: TED Radio Hour

Listen Again: Reinvention
Change is hard, but it's also an opportunity to discover and reimagine what you thought you knew. From our economy, to music, to even ourselves–this hour TED speakers explore the power of reinvention. Guests include OK Go lead singer Damian Kulash Jr., former college gymnastics coach Valorie Kondos Field, Stockton Mayor Michael Tubbs, and entrepreneur Nick Hanauer.
Now Playing: Science for the People

#562 Superbug to Bedside
By now we're all good and scared about antibiotic resistance, one of the many things coming to get us all. But there's good news, sort of. News antibiotics are coming out! How do they get tested? What does that kind of a trial look like and how does it happen? Host Bethany Brookeshire talks with Matt McCarthy, author of "Superbugs: The Race to Stop an Epidemic", about the ins and outs of testing a new antibiotic in the hospital.
Now Playing: Radiolab

Dispatch 6: Strange Times
Covid has disrupted the most basic routines of our days and nights. But in the middle of a conversation about how to fight the virus, we find a place impervious to the stalled plans and frenetic demands of the outside world. It's a very different kind of front line, where urgent work means moving slow, and time is marked out in tiny pre-planned steps. Then, on a walk through the woods, we consider how the tempo of our lives affects our minds and discover how the beats of biology shape our bodies. This episode was produced with help from Molly Webster and Tracie Hunte. Support Radiolab today at Radiolab.org/donate.