Linguists to gather in Boston for National Conference

December 26, 2012

(Washington, DC) - Hundreds of linguistic scholars from across the U.S. and around the world will convene in Boston, Massachusetts for the 87th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America (LSA) this January. Members of the news media are invited to observe and report on the proceedings.

The meeting is scheduled for January 3-6, 2013, at the Marriott Copley Place. The meeting provides a forum for the presentation of cutting-edge research focused on the scientific study of language. In addition to the LSA program, the meeting also features concurrent programs sponsored by the American Dialect Society, the American Name Society, the North American Association for the History of the Language Sciences, the Society for Pidgin and Creole Linguistics, and the Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas.

The meeting typically attracts more than 1,000 linguists who attend various sessions and workshops. Over 300 papers and 150 research posters have been approved for presentation at the meeting. For a detailed listing of all meeting sessions, please visit the LSA website: http://www.linguisticsociety.org/files/LSA2013Handbook_0.pdf.

Program Highlights

The LSA is co-sponsoring a series of joint sessions with the Modern Language Association (MLA), which will also hold its annual meeting in Boston at the same time:The LSA will also sponsor a Symposium on "Open Access and the Future of Academic Publishing" (Thursday, 3 January, 4:00 -7:00 PM), featuring Kathleen Fitzpatrick, MLA Director of Scholarly Communications.

Research Highlights

New study finds teenagers grow up linguistically

Title: The shifty vowels of African American English youth: A longitudinal study


Authors: Charlie Farrington and Mary Kohn, North Carolina State University



Teenagers are thought to use more new speech forms than adults and young children, leading many linguists to believe that they are the ones primarily responsible for historical changes in the language, including changes in pronunciation. Unless these teens are studied into adulthood, however, linguists cannot be sure that they keep their innovative forms. A recent study by Charlie Farrington and Mary Kohn of North Carolina State University tracked the speech of 22 African American youth from fourth grade until early adulthood and found that they tended to have more of these new forms in their pronunciation during middle school years compared to their earlier childhood and adult years. Although some of them kept these innovative forms into adulthood, most retreated to the pronunciations they had earlier as young children after they entered adulthood. Parents and teachers who despair that teenagers are adopting ways of speaking they disapprove of may be given some comfort. Apparently not everything they do.

Women "um" and men "uh" new study shows

Title: Gender differences in the duration of filled pauses in North American English
Author: Eric Acton, Stanford University

Previous research on men's and women's speech show that their overall rates of using the pause fillers um and uh in conversation are quite similar, although men use these fillers slightly more. A new study by Eric Acton at Stanford University shows that when um and uh are analyzed separately, there is a stark difference between women and men. Women say um rather than uh at a much higher rate than men, and this a pattern that is found across a variety of dialects of English. This difference may be due in part to differences in the way that um and uh are pronounced. An acoustic study of conversation shows that women dislike long, open-ended vowels in their pause fillers in general, causing them to opt for um, which ends in the consonant m.

Title: How are y'all/you guys/yinz going to vote?
Author: Natalie Schrimpf, affiliation?

Many linguistic studies have found that social characteristics influence speech, but a study just carried out in Middle Tennessee is the first investigation of how individual speakers' political views may be correlated with their speech. This study found that 70 speakers who voted in the 2008 presidential election showed just such a correlation with certain Southern speech features, including those of the Southern Vowel Shift. This vowel rotation includes the well-known pronunciation of the long-i vowel (as in "time") as "ah" (sounds like "Tom") and less well-known Southern pronunciation facts as the reversal of long a (as in "day" ) and short-e (as in "bet"). In this reversal "day" sounds like "die" and "bet" sounds like "bay-ut." Speakers who voted for McCain tended to show more effects of this Southern Vowel Shift than speakers who voted for Obama. The study suggests that the more Southern-oriented a speaker's vowel system is, the more likely they were to vote for McCain. The red-state orientation of the South makes this an unsurprising correlation, but the fact that Democrat voters who live in the same areas show fewer Southern speech features is an interesting new finding and reveals the subtlety of pronunciation features as they correlate with features of not just local but also personal social identity.

How low do you have to be to be male?

Title: Perceived gender in context: Gendered style among transgender speakers
Author: Lal Zimman (University of Colorado, Boulder)

We usually take for granted that a person's gender can be identified based on the pitch of his or her voice. However, a study on the speech of female-to-male transgender people by Lal Zimman of the University of Colorado suggests that the line between female- and male-sounding voices is a fuzzy one. In an experiment designed to pinpoint the precise aspects of a person's voice that allow us to identify its gender, he shows that the way a person pronounces the sibilant sound s may determine how low-pitched a voice must be before it is perceived as male. A more male-sounding s will allow a higher pitched voice to be still identified as male while a more female-sounding s will allow an even deeper voice to be identified as female.

Title: Plain English Jury Instructions for Massachusetts: first steps
Author: Janet Randall, Northeastern University

You are a juror. At the end of the trial, a judge reads the "jury instructions," to explain the laws that apply to the case. In most states the instruction about jurors' memories, says something like this:

(1) Failure of recollection is common. Innocent misrecollection is not uncommon. But in California the instruction has been revised:

(2) People often forget things or make mistakes in what they remember.

California's effort to improve jurors' understanding has been spreading to other states, and the Massachusetts Bar Association's Task Force on Plain English Jury Instructions, a group of lawyers, judges, and linguists has taken on the issue. In a series of experiments on jurors' comprehension, we are exploring whether comprehension can be significantly improved by a linguistic overhaul, the first step toward rewriting the instructions in Massachusetts. The ultimate goals of this research collaboration between linguists and the legal community are broad: to make the judicial process accessible to more citizens and to make courtroom verdicts more reliable and, ultimately, more fair.

Title: Wild sounds: extragrammatical communication in Wolof
Authors: Rebekah Baglini, Lenore Grenoble, and Martina Martinovic (University of Chicago)

'Wild sounds' refers to those that stand outside the standard set used in the ordinary pronunciation of a langauge. In English, for example, the sounds in tsk-tsk, mhmmm, and phew! do not occur in other words. In Wolof, the majority language of Senegal, wild sounds are used very frequently in casual speech. For example, speakers often click to indicate 'yes' (which is pronounced waaw in Wolof ordinarily), or purse their lips and make an elongated chirp-like click to indicate 'no' (pronounced deedeet ordinarily). A team of researchers at the University of Chicago has identified six other wild sounds (including whistles, hissing, and repeated clicks) that are used frequently in Wolof conversation to indicate agreement, understanding, disapproval, and so on, and these sounds do not occur elsewhere in the language. Wolof is not a click language like such Bantu lanaguges of South African as Xhosa and Zulu (made famous by Miraim Makeba in her popular "click song") or the Khoisan languages of southern Africa, also well-known in popular culture from the film "The Gods Must Be Crzy). In Wolof, however, clicks occur only as wild sounds, which are part of any native speaker's repertoire, but they are rarely described by linguists. Such work may help llinguists establish a better understanding of wild sounds and their relation to linguistic systems as a whole.

Awards, Honors and Related Events

In addition to the research and programmatic content presented, the meeting also features a number of awards, including the annual designation of the "Word of the Year" by the American Dialect Society, scheduled for Friday, January 4th. Other LSA awards to be presented at a special ceremony on January 5th at 5:30pm are the:Members of the Society who have made distinguished contributions to the discipline will be inducted as LSA Fellows during a ceremony at the Business Meeting, on Friday, January 4th at 5:30pm. Those being inducted are: Edwin Battistella, Southern Oregon University; Ellen Broselow, Stony Brook University; Jane Hill, University of Arizona; Hans Henrich Hock, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; Johanna B. Nichols, University of California, Berkeley; David Pesetsky, Massachusetts Institute of Technology; Dennis Preston, Oklahoma State University; Tom Roeper, University of Massachusetts; Deborah Tannen, Georgetown University. Honorary members of the LSA will also be elected at the Business Meeting.
-end-
To obtain a press badge for the meeting, please contact Alyson Reed.The Linguistic Society of America is the largest national professional society representing the field of linguistics. Its mission is to advance the scientific study of language.

Linguistic Society of America

Related Language Articles from Brightsurf:

Learning the language of sugars
We're told not to eat too much sugar, but in reality, all of our cells are covered in sugar molecules called glycans.

How effective are language learning apps?
Researchers from Michigan State University recently conducted a study focusing on Babbel, a popular subscription-based language learning app and e-learning platform, to see if it really worked at teaching a new language.

Chinese to rise as a global language
With the continuing rise of China as a global economic and trading power, there is no barrier to prevent Chinese from becoming a global language like English, according to Flinders University academic Dr Jeffrey Gil.

'She' goes missing from presidential language
MIT researchers have found that although a significant percentage of the American public believed the winner of the November 2016 presidential election would be a woman, people rarely used the pronoun 'she' when referring to the next president before the election.

How does language emerge?
How did the almost 6000 languages of the world come into being?

New research quantifies how much speakers' first language affects learning a new language
Linguistic research suggests that accents are strongly shaped by the speaker's first language they learned growing up.

Why the language-ready brain is so complex
In a review article published in Science, Peter Hagoort, professor of Cognitive Neuroscience at Radboud University and director of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, argues for a new model of language, involving the interaction of multiple brain networks.

Do as i say: Translating language into movement
Researchers at Carnegie Mellon University have developed a computer model that can translate text describing physical movements directly into simple computer-generated animations, a first step toward someday generating movies directly from scripts.

Learning language
When it comes to learning a language, the left side of the brain has traditionally been considered the hub of language processing.

Learning a second alphabet for a first language
A part of the brain that maps letters to sounds can acquire a second, visually distinct alphabet for the same language, according to a study of English speakers published in eNeuro.

Read More: Language News and Language Current Events
Brightsurf.com is a participant in the Amazon Services LLC Associates Program, an affiliate advertising program designed to provide a means for sites to earn advertising fees by advertising and linking to Amazon.com.